References

A review of clinical decision making: models and current research. J Clin Nurs. 2006; 17:(2)187-95

Bajwa SU, Kitchlew N, Shahzad K, Rehman KU Brief communication: phronesis knowledge as enabler of intuitive decision making. Knowledge Organization. 2015; 42:(1)40-9

Barber K The complexity of decision-making in midwifery: A case study. British Journal of Midwifery. 2012; 20:(4)289-94 https://doi.org/10.12968/bjom.2012.20.4.289

Barnfather T Can intuitive knowledge be taught in midwifery practice?. British Journal of Midwifery. 2013; 21:(2)131-6 https://doi.org/10.12968/bjom.2013.21.2.131

Benner P From novice to expert. Am J Nurs. 1982; 82:(3)402-7

Benner PNew Jersey: Prentice Hall; 2000

Benner P, Tanner C Clinical judgment: how expert nurses use intuition. Am J Nurs. 1987; 87:(1)23-31

Blumenthal-Barby JS, Krieger H Cognitive biases and heuristics in medical decision making: a critical review using a systematic search strategy. Med Decis Making. 2015; 35:(4)539-57 https://doi.org/10.1177/0272989X14547740

Carper B Fundamental patterns of knowing in nursing. ANS Adv Nurs Sci. 1978; 1:(1)13-23

Cervone HF Systematic vs intuitive decision making and the Pareto principle: Effective decision-making for project teams. OCLC Systems & Services: International digital library perspectives. 2015; 31:(3)108-11 https://doi.org/10.1108/OCLC-05-2015-0005

Cook DA Much ado about differences: why expert-novice comparisons add little to the validity argument. Adv Health Sci Educ Theory Pract. 2015; 20:(3)829-34 https://doi.org/10.1007/s10459-014-9551-3

Cross RECheshire: Books for Midwives; 1996

Dale S Heuristics and biases: The science of decision-making. Business Information Review. 2015; 32:(2)93-9 https://doi.org/10.1177/0266382115592536

Draper ES, Kurinczuk JJ, Kenyon S(eds.). Leicester: MBRRACE-UK; 2015

Dreyfus SE, Dreyfus HLUniversity of California, Berkeley: Operations Research Centre; 1980

Elstein A, Shulman L, Sprafka SCambridge: Harvard University Press; 1978

Freeman LM, Griew K Enhancing the midwife–woman relationship through shared decision making and clinical guidelines. Women Birth. 2007; 20:(1)11-15 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wombi.2006.10.003

Goodman B, Ley TLondon: SAGE Publications; 2012

Griffith R Understanding accountability in midwifery practice: Key concepts. British Journal of Midwifery. 2011; 19:(5)327-8 https://doi.org/10.12968/bjom.2011.19.5.327

Griffith R Accountability in midwifery practice: answerable to mother and baby. British Journal of Midwifery. 2012; 20:(8)601-2 https://doi.org/10.12968/bjom.2012.20.8.601

HM Government. The Nursing and Midwifery Order. 2001. http://tinyurl.com/np6cg46 (accessed 14 July 2016)

Jefford E, Fahy K, Sundin D Decision-Making Theories and their usefulness to the midwifery profession both in terms of midwifery practice and the education of midwives. Int J Nurs Pract. 2011; 17:(3)246-53 https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1440-172X.2010.01900.x

Jordan B Authoritative knowledge and its construction.(eds.). Berkeley: University of California Press; 1997

Kahneman DLondon: Penguin; 2011

Kennedy C, O’Reilly P, Fealy G, Casey M, Brady AM, McNamara M Comparative analysis of nursing and midwifery regulatory and professional bodies’ scope of practice and associated decision-making frameworks: a discussion paper. J Adv Nurs. 2015; 71:(8)1797-811 https://doi.org/10.1111/jan.12660

Klein G A naturalistic decision making perspective on studying intuitive decision making. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition. 2015; 4:(3)164-68 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2015.07.001

Klein G, Calderwood R, Clinton-Cirocco A Rapid Decision Making on the Fire Ground. Proc Hum Fact Ergon Soc Annu Meet. 1988; 30:(6)576-80

Knight M, Tuffnell D, Kenyon S, Shakespeare J, Gray R, Kurinczuk JJ(eds.). Oxford: National Perinatal Epidemiology Unit; 2015

Ménage D Part 1: A model for evidence-based decision-making in midwifery care. British Journal of Midwifery. 2016a; 24:(1)44-9 https://doi.org/10.12968/bjom.2016.24.1.44

Ménage D Part 2: A model for evidence-based decision-making in midwifery care. British Journal of Midwifery. 2016b; 24:(2)137-43 https://doi.org/10.12968/bjom.2016.24.2.137

McFall JP Rational, normative, descriptive, prescriptive, or choice behavior? The search for integrative metatheory of decision making. Behavioral Development Bulletin. 2015; 20:(1)45-59 https://doi.org/10.1037/h0101039

Mok H, Stevens PA Models of decision making.(eds.). Philadelphia: Elsevier; 2005

Muoni T Decision-making, intuition, and the midwife: Understanding heuristics. British Journal of Midwifery. 2012; 20:(1)52-6 https://doi.org/10.12968/bjom.2012.20.1.52

National Institute for Health and Care Excellence. Intrapartum care for healthy women and babies CG190. 2014. http://www.nice.org.uk/guidance/cg190 (accessed 14 July 2016)

London: NMC; 2012

London: NMC; 2015

Okoli JO, Weller G, Watt J Information processing and intuitive decision-making on the fireground: towards a model of expert intuition. Cognition Technology and Work. 2016; 18:(1)89-103 https://doi.org/10.1007/s10111-015-0348-9

Payne LK Toward a theory of intuitive decision-making in nursing. Nurs Sci Q. 2015; 28:(3)223-8 https://doi.org/10.1177/0894318415585618

Raynor M, Marshall J, Sullivan A(eds.). Philadelphia: Elsevier; 2005

Rew L Acknowledging intuition in clinical decision making. J Holist Nurs. 2000; 18:(2)94-108 https://doi.org/10.1177/089801010001800202

Standing MMaidenhead: Open University Press; 2010

Thompson C, Dowding D Decision making and judgement in nursing – an introduction.(eds.). London: Churchill Livingstone; 2002

Decision-making in midwifery: A tripartite clinical decision

02 August 2016
Volume 24 · Issue 8

Abstract

Decision-making within midwifery practice is complex and challenging, and is directly linked to the standard of care provided. In the literature around decision-making there are alternative approaches mentioned, which are founded on the notion that decision-making is either: logical, such as Elstein et al's (1978) four-stage model; intuitive, such as the intuitive-humanistic model; or an amalgamation of the two. The extent to which these existing models can be applied to midwifery decision-making is discussed, and an adapted decision-making model is presented. This modified model attempts to address the complexity of decision-making in midwifery practice, with reference to a clinical case study to illustrate its function.

This paper aims to consider clinical decision-making within midwifery and critically analyse three decision-making approaches—the hypothetico-deductive approach, the intuitive-humanistic approach and the dual process theory—in relation to a chosen case. Using a modified decision-making tool, a tripartite decision (Box 1) will be discussed and the key elements involved in decision-making within midwifery explored. Pseudonyms have been used when discussing the tripartite decision in order to maintain confidentiality (Nursing and Midwifery Council (NMC), 2015).

Case study: A tripartite decision in midwifery practice

Laura*, a woman at term, was visited at home for an early labour assessment. On arrival she appeared to be experiencing one uterine contraction every 7 minutes. Contractions were moderate to palpate, but did not seem to be causing her too much discomfort. The rationale for a vaginal examination to assess her cervical effacement and dilatation was discussed and the woman requested this procedure. Upon examination, it was found that the cervix was mid-anterior, soft and stretchy, 0.5 cm long and 4 cm dilated, which could be stretched to 6 cm. Although 4 cm dilated, the woman's uterine contractions were incoordinate, and too lacking in strength and frequency to diagnose active labour.

The woman appeared to be anxious and lacking in confidence, and expressed her concern regarding going to hospital as she feared intervention. She had previously experienced a Neville Barnes forceps delivery, a postpartum haemorrhage and a retained placenta that required manual removal in theatre.

The tripartite decision was made by the midwife, the woman and the student midwife that the woman was in early labour and should, therefore, remain at home and await events.

*Name changed to protect confidentiality

There is a great deal of controversy over the definition of what a decision is (Raynor et al, 2005). Cross (1996) takes the perspective that a decision is the point at which a course of action is determined, with the internal processes by which a course of action or inaction is chosen known as decision-making (McFall, 2015). Clinical decision-making is a process that is fundamental to good professional practice (Thompson and Dowding, 2002). However, the complexity of the decision-making process is well known (Mok and Stevens, 2005) and within the clinical setting decisions must be made, frequently with missing or ambiguous information (Rew, 2000). The quality of the care that is provided by midwives is directly related to the quality of the decision-making process (Jefford, 2012). Poor decision-making is a theme evident in confidential enquiries and audit reports, indicating that poor clinical decision-making contributes to poor maternal and neonatal outcomes (Draper et al, 2015; Knight et al, 2015). In 2001, the Nursing and Midwifery Order stated that there was to be a professional body, the NMC, that would determine standards of education, training, conduct and performance for midwives and ensure that these standards are maintained (HM Government, 2001). In turn, the NMC outlines a midwife's scope of practice in order to determine procedures, actions and processes which midwives are licenced to perform, in order to regulate the decision-making process (Kennedy et al, 2015).

Decision-making models

The plethora of decision-making models and tools available to aid an individual in their decision-making may be categorised into three theoretical approaches: normative, descriptive and prescriptive. Table 1 highlights the differences between the three approaches.


Approach Explanation Application
Normative This approach assumes the individual is rational and logical, and is concerned with how decisions should be made. What is important is how ‘good’ the outcome is Social judgement theory; Baye's decision tree (Thompson and Dowding, 2002)
Descriptive This approach describes how individuals reach their decisions and is process-focused Information-processing theory; Elstein's four-stage process
Prescriptive This approach attempts to improve decisions Cognitive continuum; National Institute for Health and Care Excellence clinical guidelines
Thompson and Dowding (2002); Standing (2010)

Most decision-making models appear to focus on decision-making being either rational and logical, or intuitive. This notion that there are two cognitive routes is described by Kahneman (2011), who proposes that individuals possess two ‘thinking’ systems, namely system 1 and system 2 – terms coined by psychologists Stanovich and West (Kahneman, 2011). According to Kahneman (2011), system 1 is quick to act and does so without great (or any) effort, whereas system 2 purposefully directs attention to cognitive activities. Kahneman implies that although separate systems, the two processes can work cooperatively as system 1 generates impressions or feelings about stimuli/situations and system 2 then uses these to create explicit beliefs and deliberate choices. However, popular decision-making theories tend to refer to the decision-making process involving only one of these two systems. For instance, analytical approaches claim that clinical reasoning, which follows rational logic akin to system 2, contributes to clinical decision-making (Banning, 2006), while intuitive/humanistic approaches, similar to system 1, claim intuition is the process by which decisions are made (Ménage, 2016a).

One branch of analytical/rational decision-making is the hypothetico-deductive theory, thought to be the dominant decision-making approach within health sciences (Jefford et al, 2011). Based on the descriptive analysis of reasoning by medical interns, Elstein et al (1978) proposed that the reasoning process comprises four stages (Table 2).


Stage Description Application
Cue aquisition The collection of information History-taking, observation
Hypothesis generation Retrieval of problem formulations from memory Potential diagnosis e.g. early labour
Cue interpretation Interpretation of information in light of diagnosis under consideration Physical examinations e.g. internal vaginal examination
Hypothesis evaluation Weighing and combining of information to confirm or deny diagnostic hypothesis Confirm or deny hypothesis of early labour in light of findings
Elstein et al (1978)

This systematic approach to decision-making supports the use of the four-stage model within clinical practice, as the agreed process allows for others to review the stages and verify clinicians' knowledge and reasoning (Jefford et al, 2011). The objective nature of the model is congruent with the medicalisation of obstetric-led maternity care (Goodman and Ley, 2012) and the step-by-step process can be applied to everyday decisions. However, the four-stage model focuses purely on the objective and observable cues available to clinicians and does not consider the role of emotion or beliefs and values, all of which play a part in holistic midwifery care and woman-centred decision-making (Barber, 2012).

In light of the criticisms of a hypothetico-deductive approach to decision-making, an alternative intuitive-humanistic theoretical approach may be considered. Although it has previously been argued that intuition forms the basis of guesswork, intuition is now recognised as an essential factor in clinical judgement (Benner and Tanner, 1987). Barnfather (2013) describes how tacit knowledge arises from accumulating a range of different forms of information, including explicit knowledge, aesthetics, self-awareness and personal knowledge—forms of knowledge similar to Carper's (1978) four elements. Applying this approach to the tripartite decision in Box 1, the midwife would make a timely decision regarding Laura's stage of labour by combining her forms of knowledge. An intuitive-humanistic approach prioritises emotion and also allows clinicians to make decisions promptly (Jefford et al, 2011). The use of intuition in clinical decision-making is, however, subjective because it is based on past experience and so can lead to errors in decision-making (Ménage, 2016a).

The dual process theory attempts to combine the use of analytical, rational thought and intuition by proposing that system 1 is responsible for cue acquisition and interpretation, while system 2 is responsible when no existing memory pattern is available. However, Jefford et al (2011) highlight that the dual process theory does not indicate how clinicians move between intuitive and analytic modes of decision-making.

Alternative model

There is limited evidence regarding decision-making in midwifery, so the multitude of influencing factors involved in midwifery decision-making are currently not considered in an existing decision-making model. Therefore, a descriptive decision-making model is proposed, which includes a modified version of Elstein et al's (1978) four-stage process that explains the roles of both system 1 and system 2, and includes the consideration of individual circumstances (Figure 1). The highlighted boxes represent changes or additions to the original four-stage model, and will be elaborated on in this article.

Figure 1. Modified decision-making tool combining Elstein et al (1978) and Kahneman (2001) to aid decision-making in midwifery practice

The first two stages occur intuitively and through system 1 thinking. Based on Klein's (2015) distinction, the midwife in the case study used pattern recognition rather than heuristics to arrive at hypothesis generation (Figure 2).

Figure 2. Stages 1 and 2 of the modified decision-making model

Owing to the professional accountability a midwife has and the trusting relationship formed between the woman and the midwife, the midwife engages her rational, analytic system (system 2) in order to ensure the appropriateness of her hypothesis (Figure 3).

Figure 3. Stage 3 of the modified decision-making model

The additional feature in this modified decision-making tool that is fundamental to midwifery is the consideration of individual circumstances—for example, the values, preferences and wishes of the woman. This stage ensures that woman-centred care is provided, and that the individual needs of the woman are considered (Ménage, 2016b) (Figure 4).

Figure 4. Stages 4 and 5 of the modified decision-making model

This model considers the pivotal role that intuition plays in the experienced professional's decision-making process, while acknowledging the importance of rational thinking in order to make beneficent decisions. The use of Elstein's model allows the logical sequence of decision-making to be maintained, with the addition of a further stage that epitomises the holistic midwifery care to be provided.

Intuition

A key component of the proposed modified model is the role of intuition in recognising pattern formations to arrive at a hypothesis. The definition of intuition in terms of decision-making is ambiguous and, therefore, intuitive decision-making may be seen as substandard (Payne, 2015). However, despite the midwife in the case study being unable to articulate her intuitive response regarding Laura's condition, her practical wisdom—or phronesis (Bajwa et al, 2015)—was based on more than ‘gut feeling’. Klein (2015) outlined two alternative views concerning the basis of intuition. The notion of fast and frugal heuristics (Klein, 2015) regards heuristics as the foundation of intuitive thinking. Heuristics—mental shortcuts to simplify information (Muoni, 2012)—can be applied quickly to situations in order to arrive at decisions, although they are generic (Klein, 2015) and subject to biases (Dale, 2015). Blumenthal-Barby and Krieger (2015) argued that many studies investigating the use of heuristics in medical decision-making use hypothetical situations and, therefore, evidence regarding the use of heuristics in real-life decision-making is lacking. An alternative viewpoint, held by naturalistic decision-making (NDM) theorists—who attempted to understand how individuals make decisions in applied, natural settings as opposed to laboratory settings—is that of intuition as an expression of pattern recognition (Klein, 2015). Klein et al (1988) examined the way in which fire ground commanders made decisions under extreme pressure and detailed the recognition-primed decision model, which focused on the use of pattern recognition rather than systematic calculation for rapid decision-making. Klein (2015) explains how individuals match a given situation to patterns they have learned and follow the expected course of action, allowing for prompt decision-making. In contrast to heuristics, patterns are more specific and are representations of tacit knowledge (Klein, 2015), the kind of knowledge fundamental to intuition. It is this pattern recognition that describes the intuitive process undertaken in stages 1 and 2 of the modified model, as specific tools are required for clinical decision-making in midwifery to avoid errors and biases. The NDM approach of the modified model strengthens the intuitive argument as it recognises the high level of experience and tacit knowledge required for the midwife to recognise Laura's behaviour pattern and make a prompt judgement. Crucially, Cervone (2015) argued that 80% of decisions are not strategic and lack importance, so the use of intuition is sufficient; however, he goes on to suggest that for the 20% of decisions that have a real impact, more systematic and rational decision-making methods should be employed. This notion discourages the use of intuition in an early labour assessment, as such a decision has a large impact on the care the woman will subsequently receive. The modified model overcomes this issue by suggesting that both intuition and reasoning are to be used in conjunction.

Intuition at varying levels of competence

Benner (1982; 2000) argues that it is only the expert who is able to accurately use his or her intuition to guide decision-making. The Dreyfus model of skill acquisition (Dreyfus and Dreyfus, 1980), based on the skill acquisition of pilots and chess players, proposes that a student progresses through five levels of competency, from a novice to an expert. Benner applied this model to nursing and, through interviews and participant observations, arrived at a description of the passage of the nurses from novice to expert (Table 3).


Level Application to nursing Application to midwifery
Novice No experience of situations in which the nurse is expected to carry out tasks. Performance governed by context-free rules which are inflexible and limited First-year student midwife who is restricted to theoretical knowledge, or a primigravid woman who has no tangible experience of early labour
Advanced beginner Encountered enough real-life situations to recognise important characteristics (which Dreyfus calls aspects). The advanced beginner requires support in prioritising care as still reliant on generic guidelines Second-year student midwife or a multiparous woman who has experienced/witnessed similar situations previously but lacks the foresight to set priorities
Competent Typically a nurse who has worked for 2–3 years and is aware of which aspects of a situation are more important; however, still requires conscious, deliberate planning Third-year student midwife or newly qualified midwife who is able to formulate plans with a long-term goal in mind but decision-making is still a calculated, lengthy process
Proficient Views situations as a whole and recognises deviation from normality, hones in on most important feature. Recognition of maxims (undetectable differences within situations) separates competent from proficient The proficient midwife is an experienced practitioner able to promptly and accurately make decisions, owing to a deep understanding of the situation
Expert No longer relies on conceptual framework to comprehend situation, focuses on integral part of situation. Does not solely rely on intuitive element as during new situations analytic reasoning may be required The expert midwife uses this intuition obtained through experience and formal education to guide decision-making
Benner (1982); Standing (2010)

In terms of the tripartite decision outlined in Box 1, the three individuals involved were at varying levels of ability. In this scenario, the midwife's expertise was weighed against the student midwife's and woman's lack of experience in making such decisions. The student midwife demonstrated competent characteristics, as the National Institute for Health and Care Excellence (NICE, 2014) guidance regarding definitions of the latent and established first stage of labour were considered. The student midwife focused on the findings from the vaginal examinations, but consideration of other significant factors was slow and deliberate and this process was not intuitive, as it occurred after the consideration of available information. The cognitive architecture of the experienced midwife allowed her to initially assess the situation and access her knowledge (Okoli et al, 2016) to hone in on the important feature—which she considered to be the lack of strong, regular, painful contractions—and she therefore hypothesised that Laura was, in fact, in early labour. The midwife, as the expert, was able to access a wide range of stored patterns and generate the most plausible course of action (Klein, 2015). Although the novice-to-expert continuum appears applicable to midwifery (Table 3), Cook (2015) highlights a flaw in comparison studies that appear to support the novice-to-expert distinction. Cook argues that there are various explanations for observed differences between novices and experts, and these confounding variables reduce the validity of any novice-to-expert studies, such as those by Benner (1982; 2000). A further criticism of the novice-to-expert argument is that the Dreyfus model and Benner's work are now outdated, so many contemporary driving forces in clinical decision-making may not be considered within the model.

Systematic reasoning

The second phase of the modified decision-making model outlined above involves systematic, rational thought. This is required to ensure that initial intuitive-based hypotheses are tested and other factors are considered before arriving at a decision, to ensure safe and holistic care is delivered. It is necessary for the midwife to engage in this analytic processing because of the responsibility and accountability he or she has. Accountability is defined as having to answer to a higher authority for one's actions, whereas responsibility refers to an individual's authority over someone else (Griffith, 2011). In this tripartite scenario, the midwife and student midwife had a responsibility to consider Laura's values and priorities when making a decision about her plan of care. However, the midwife was also accountable for the diagnosis of early labour; accountable to Laura, to society as a whole, to her own employer and to the profession (the NMC) (Griffith, 2011). The NMC (2012) outlines in its Midwives Rules and Standards that a practising midwife is responsible for providing care to a woman and that through doing this, the needs of the woman and her baby are the midwife's primary focus. The NMC (2015)Code states that midwives must be accountable for their decisions and, if this is not adhered to, a midwife's fitness to practise is called into question. Midwives have a duty of care (Griffith, 2012) to ensure no harm is caused through their practice. As accountable and responsible professionals, the midwife and the student midwife discussed with Laura the evidence-based guidance and her high-risk labour status, while also considering Laura's fear of intervention during her labour. It is these more deliberate thought processes and discussions that require rational, systematic thought, and ensure shared decision-making is achieved through the sharing of information.

Jordan (1997) talks of authoritative knowledge and how frequently one kind of knowledge gains superiority and dismisses other forms of ‘knowing’. This model balances different forms of knowledge and allows evidence-based knowledge and emotional intelligence to be weighted equally. Through this equal weighting of clinical and individual needs, shared decision-making is achieved (Freeman and Griew, 2007). In the case study in Box 1, the midwife was able to progress through all the stages of decision-making and consider all of these aspects in order to arrive at a decision in partnership with the student midwife and Laura. If in a similar situation again in clinical practice, the modified tool detailed in this article would be helpful to ensure all important features of the decision-making process are considered, while giving weight to the intuition the expert possesses. If a member of the decision-making team lacks the expertise required for successful intuition, the process can occur more systematically, allowing the individual to gain experience and, in turn, develop his/her intuition.

Conclusion

There are a range of decision-making approaches and theories that outline different methods for arriving at a decision. The holistic nature of midwifery and the lack of current evidence regarding midwifery decision-making mean that a modified tool is necessary to demonstrate how clinical decisions are made. The midwife possesses the expertise to use his or her intuition to guide the decision-making process; however, as responsible and accountable professionals, it is necessary for midwives to follow-up intuitive thought with rational, systematic processing. Through the combination of intuition and rationality, and the consideration of evidence as well as individual emotions, values and beliefs, clinicians are able to successfully make shared clinical decisions.

Key points

  • In midwifery practice, complex decisions often have to be made with missing or ambiguous information
  • There are a plethora of decision-making models that have attempted to explain how or why people make decisions
  • There is limited evidence regarding decision-making in midwifery and a lack of midwifery-specific decision-making models
  • Intuition can be described as pattern recognition, as individuals are able to make prompt decisions by recognising patterns and reacting accordingly
  • Midwives are required to engage in rational thought during the decision-making process, owing to the responsibility and accountability they possess
  • Evidence-based knowledge and emotional intelligence must be balanced in order to meet women's needs